Изображения страниц
PDF
EPUB

Shefner, S. Electrophysiological effects of ethanol

on brain neurons. In: Watson, R.R., ed. Bio-
chemistry and Physiology of Substance Abuse.
Vol. 2. Boca Raton, Fla.: CRC Press, 1990.
pp. 25–53.

Simson, P.E.; Criswell, H.E.; Johnson, K.B.; Hicks,
R.E.; and Breese, G.R. Ethanol inhibits NMDA-
evoked electro-physiological activity in vivo.
J Pharmacol Exp Ther 257(1):225–231, 1991.
Sinclair, J.G., and Lo, G.F. Ethanol blocks tetanic
and calcium-induced long-term potentiation in
the hippocampal slice. Gen Pharmacol
17(2):231–233, 1986.

Skattebol, A., and Rabin, R.A. Effects of ethanol on 45Ca2+ uptake in synaptosomes and in PC12 cells. Biochem Pharmacol 36(13):2227–2229, 1987.

Sladeczek, F.; Pin, J.P.; Recasens, M.; Bockaert, J.;
and Weiss, S. Glutamate stimulates inositol phos-
phate formation in striatal neurons. Nature
317(6039):717-719, 1985.

Staubli, U.; Thibault, O.; DiLorenzo, M.; and Lynch,
G. Antagonism of NMDA receptors impairs ac-
quisition but not retention of olfactory memory.
Behav Neurosci 103(1):54-60, 1989.
Suzdak, P.D., and Paul, S.M. Ethanol stimulates
GABA receptor-mediated Cl- ion flux in vitro:
Possible relationship to the anxiolytic and intoxi-
cating actions of alcohol. Psychopharmacol
Bull 23(3):445-451, 1987.

Suzdak, P.D.; Schwartz, R.D.; Skolnick, P.; and Paul, S.M. Ethanol stimulates y-aminobutyric acid receptor-mediated chloride transport in rat brain synaptoneurosomes. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 83:4071-4075, 1986.

Szabo, G.; Tabakoff, B.; and Hoffman, P.L. Receptors with V1 characteristics mediate the maintenance of ethanol tolerance by vasopressin. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 247(2):536–541, 1988. Tabakoff, B.; Whelan, J.P.; and Hoffman, P.L. Two biological markers of alcoholism. In: Cloninger, C.R., and Begleiter, H., eds. Genetics and Biology of Alcoholism: Banbury Report 33. New York: Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press, 1990. pp. 195–204.

Ticku, M.K. Alcohol and GABA-benzodiazepine receptor function. Ann Med 22(4):241–246, 1990. Tomkins, D.M.; Sellers, E.M.; and Higgins, G.A. Effect of serotonin antagonists on the dexfenfluramine (D)-induced attenuation of ethanol intake in Wistar rats. Soc Neurosci Abstracts 17:1423, 1991.

Turner, E.; Ewing, J.; Shilling, P.; Smith, T.L.; Irwin, M.; Schuckit, M.; and Kelsoe, J.R. Lack of association between an RFLP near the D2 dopamine

receptor gene and severe alcoholism. Biol Psychiatry 31:285–290, 1992. Valverius, P.; Borg, S.; Valverius, M.R.; Hoffman, P.L.; and Tabakoff, B. Beta-adrenergic receptor binding in brain of alcoholics. Exp Neurol 105(3):280-286, 1989.

Valverius, P.; Crabbe, J.C.; Hoffman, P.L.; and Tabakoff, B. NMDA receptors in mice bred to be

prone or resistant to ethanol withdrawal seizures. Eur J Pharmacol 184(1):185–189, 1990. Valverius, P.; Hoffman, P.L.; and Tabakoff, B. Effect of ethanol on mouse cerebral cortical betaadrenergic receptors. Mol Pharmacol 32(1): 217-222, 1987.

Waddington, J.L. Sight and insight: Brain dopamine receptor occupancy by neuroleptics visualised in living schizophrenic patients by positron emission tomography. Br J Psychiatry 154:433–436,

1989.

Wafford, K.A.; Burnett, D.M.; Dunwiddie, T.V.; and Harris, R.A. Genetic differences in the ethanol sensitivity of GABAA receptors expressed in Xenopus oocytes. Science 249(103):291–293, 1990.

Wafford, K.A.; Burnett, D.M.; Leidenheimer, N.J.;

Burt, D.R.; Wang, J.B.; Kofuji, P.; Dunwiddie, T.V.; Harris, R.A.; and Sikela, J.M. Ethanol sensitivity of the GABAA receptor expressed in Xenopus oocytes requires 8 amino acids contained in the gamma 2L subunit. Neuron 7(1):27-33, 1991.

Walker, D.W., and Zornetzer, S.F. Alcohol withdrawal in mice: Electroencephalographic and behavioral correlates. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 36(3):233–243, 1974.

Walter, R.; van Ree, J.M.; and de Wied, D. Modification of conditioned behavior of rats by neurohypophyseal hormones and analogues. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 75(5):2493–2496, 1978. Wang, X.M.; Lemos, J.R.; Dayanithi, G.; Nordmann, J.J.; and Treistman, S.N. Ethanol reduces vasopressin release by inhibiting calcium currents in nerve terminals. Brain Res 551(1–2):338–341, 1991.

Watkins, J.C., and Olverman, H.J. Agonists and antagonists for excitatory amino acid receptors. Trends Neurosci 10(7):265–272, 1987. White, G.; Lovinger, D.M.; and Weight, F.F. Ethanol inhibits NMDA-activated current but does not alter GABA-activated current in an isolated adult mammalian neuron. Brain Res 507(2):332–336, 1990.

Whiting, P.; McKernan, R.M.; and Iversen, L.L. Another mechanism for creating diversity in gamma-aminobutyrate type A receptors: RNA

splicing directs expression of two forms of gamma 2 subunit, one of which contains a protein kinase C phosphorylation site. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 87(24):9966-9970, 1990. Whittington, M.A., and Little, H.J. Patterns of changes in field potentials in the isolated hippocampal slice on withdrawal from chronic ethanol treatments of mice in vivo. Brain Res 523(2):237-244, 1990.

Whittington, M.A., and Little, H.J. Nitrendipine, given during drinking, decreases the electrophysiological changes in the isolated hippocampal slice, seen during ethanol withdrawal. Brj Pharmacol 103(3):1677-1684, 1991. Wilkinson, D.A. CT scan and neuropsychological assessments of alcoholism. In: Parsons, O.A.; Butters, N.; and Nathan, P.E., eds. Neuropsychology of Alcoholism: Implications for Diagnosis

and Treatment. New York: Guilford Press,
1987. pp. 76–102.

Wilson, W.R.; Bosy, T.Z.; and Ruth, J.A. NMDA agonists and antagonists alter the hypnotic response to ethanol in LS and SS mice. Alcobol 7(5):389–395, 1990.

Woodward, J.J., and Gonzales, R.A. Ethanol inhibition of N-methyl-D-aspartate-stimulated endogenous dopamine release from rat striatal slices: Reversal by glycine. J Neurochem 54(2):712715, 1990.

Wozniak, K.M.; Pert, A.; and Linnoila, M. Antagonism of 5-HT3 receptors attenuates the effects of ethanol on extracellular dopamine. Eur J Pharmacol 187(2):287–289, 1990.

Yakel, J.L., and Jackson, M.B. 5-HT3 receptors mediate rapid responses in cultured hippocampus and a clonal cell line. Neuron 1(7):615–621, 1988.

CHAPTER

5

NEUROBEHAVIORAL ASPECTS OF ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION

Introduction

W

hat behavioral effects are produced by alcohol? Do changes in the brain occur along with these effects? Answers to these pivotal questions in alcohol research should provide some knowledge of the mechanisms that underlie the behavioral effects of alcohol and insight into why some people seek repeated exposure to alcohol despite adverse consequences. Such alcohol-seeking behavior appears to contribute significantly to the development of chronic drinking.

Chronic alcohol consumption can result in physical and psychological dependence. Physical dependence is indicated by signs of withdrawal that result from the absence of alcohol in the body when drinking is discontinued. An individual suffering from alcohol withdrawal may experience a host of symptoms ranging from profound anxiety, tremors, hyperthermia, and sleep disturbances to hallucinations and seizures. It has been hypothesized that physically dependent individuals drink to avoid or alleviate these aversive symptoms of withdrawal. However, as clinicians can attest, abstinent alcoholics often resume drinking alcohol long after the signs of physical dependence have disappeared. This fact suggests that recovering alcoholics who resume drinking do so for reasons other than to alleviate withdrawal symptoms. Studies have shown that animals that are repeatedly given doses of alcohol sufficient to produce physical dependence do not necessarily begin to self-administer alcohol voluntarily once the forced alcohol adminis

tration is discontinued (Cappell and LeBlanc 1981; Winger 1988). These and other studies indicate that physical dependence is not a requisite condition for the initiation of voluntary alcohol self-administration (Deneau et al. 1969). In fact, physical dependence is viewed now as an important adverse medical consequence of excessive alcohol consumption rather than as a primary cause of abusive drinking.

Researchers have begun to examine the reinforcing properties of drugs in their efforts to understand the processes responsible for alcohol-seeking behavior.

In contrast to physical dependence, psychological dependence is characterized by compulsive craving and drinking that is independent of the presence or absence of physical dependence and withdrawal (Eddy et al. 1965). Studies with other drugs of abuse have substantiated the concept of psychological dependence. For example, Jaffe (1985) reported that although cocaine does not produce pronounced physical dependence, it does produce marked psychological dependence and craving.

Researchers have begun to examine the reinforcing properties of drugs in their efforts to understand the processes responsible for alcohol-seeking behavior. The term reinforcement describes the process whereby the probability of a response (e.g., drug-taking behavior)

Figure 1. Rat in a Skinner box responding for brain stimulation reward.

This rat is pressing a lever to deliver a small electric current to the tip of an electrode surgically implanted in its brain. The study of brain stimulation reward potentially can provide information about what neurochemical systems are involved in alcohol and other drug abuse.

Drawing courtesy of Robert Czechowski and Clare Little.

is increased if it results in the attainment of a particular effect (Skinner 1938, 1953). An individual learns a behavior, such as drinking alcohol, to obtain a particular outcome or reinforcer, such as the euphoric effect or the anxiolytic effect of alcohol. This chapter focuses on the neurobehavioral aspects of alcohol reinforcement. A detailed review of significant findings in studies of the neurophysiological, neuroanatomical, and neuropharmacological effects and actions of alcohol is found in Chapter 4, Actions of Alcohol on the Brain.

Alcohol as a Reinforcer: Brain Mechanisms of

Reinforcement

One theory of the biological or physiological basis for alcohol reinforcement suggests that drugs are self-administered if they affect a specific reinforcement system in the brain. For example, many people state that consuming alcohol is a

pleasant experience; yet researchers have questioned whether the pleasant experience stems from alcohol-induced euphoria or from the alleviation of an underlying anxiety.

Brain stimulation reward (BSR), also known as intracranial self-stimulation (Olds and Milner 1954), is an experimental procedure that has been used to study the neurobehavioral basis of the euphoric properties of alcohol and other drugs of abuse. In these studies, animals will readily learn to deliver small levels of electric current to specific areas of the brain to elicit rewarding stimulation (figure 1).

Study results suggest that the neural structures related to BSR may be a specialized system responsible for the process of reinforcement (Olds and Fobes 1981). It is postulated that drugs of abuse function as reinforcers by imitating, facilitating, or sometimes blocking the various neurochemical messengers (e.g., neurotransmitters) and neural modulators involved in this system (Koob and Bloom 1988; Wise 1980, 1987). Experimental evidence supports this theory: Wise (1987) has reported that drugs of abuse, including psychomotor stimulants (e.g., amphetamine and cocaine) and opiates (e.g., morphine and heroin), facilitate BSR. The administration of these drugs either increases the rate at which the animal self-stimulates (i.e., presses a bar to receive more stimulation) or lowers the threshold for BSR so that a smaller amount of electrical current is required to sustain the same self-stimulation behavior, or both. If an animal's response rate increases after alcohol administration, it may be concluded that alcohol increases the reward, or euphoria, experienced by the animal. A decrease in response rate may indicate diminished reward.

[graphic]

Alcohol's Effects on Brain
Stimulation Reward

The effects of alcohol on BSR are not as unambig-
uous as those of stimulants and opiates. Indeed,
researchers have questioned whether changes in
response rate observed after alcohol administra-
tion stem from the effect of alcohol on motor sys-
tems rather than from alteration in the reward
value of the stimulation. Accordingly, several
investigators have measured changes in BSR
threshold in place of response rate to determine
whether alcohol can facilitate BSR performance
(Kornetsky et al. 1979; Lewis and Phelps 1987).
This approach also presents challenges, because

« ПредыдущаяПродолжить »