Изображения страниц
PDF
EPUB

for alcohol's anxiolytic actions has focused on the involvement of the GABA-benzodiazepine receptor complex (see chapter 4). Interest in this receptor complex is partially the result of the discovery that the experimental drug RO 15-4513 can counteract some of alcohol's intoxicating effects (Lister 1987; Suzdak et al. 1988) by its actions at the GABA-benzodiazepine receptor complex. RO 15-4513 reduces the effectiveness of alcohol as an anxiolytic (Koob et al. 1986; Lister 1988). Thus, alcohol may act on the same system as the benzodiazepines, which are commonly prescribed anxiolytic drugs (e.g., Librium). RO 15-4513 and the chemically similar drugs FG 7142 and RO 19-4603 can reduce alcohol intake (Balakleevsky et al. 1990; June et al. 1991; Samson et al. 1989), but other effects of these drugs make them unsuitable as pharmacological agents for treating alcohol abuse and alcoholism.

The aversive properties of alcohol are relevant to alcohol abuse for two reasons: First, they may place an upper limit on the amount of alcohol that can be consumed, and second, tolerance to the aversive effects of alcohol may account for increases in alcohol intake over time.

The 5-HT system has also been implicated in the control of anxiety (Chopin and Briley 1987). Some of the same serotonin-related drugs that reduce alcohol self-administration (e.g., buspirone) also reduce anxiety (Dunn et al. 1989) or alter the anxiolytic effects of alcohol (Durcan et al. 1988). The selectively bred P and NP rats, which differ in GABA and serotonergic functioning (McBride et al. 1990), also differ in sensitivity to the anxiolytic effects of alcohol; NP rats show reduced anxiety at lower doses of alcohol than the P rats (Baldwin et al. 1991).

The effects of alcohol on anxiety in rats have also been studied by using stressors that are more likely to occur in a rat's natural environment, for example, the presence of a predator (Blanchard et al. 1990). Reductions in anxiety may be correlated with increased aggressiveness. Pretreatment with low doses of alcohol was found to increase the attack behavior of male and female rats toward male intruders (Blanchard, Flannelly et al. 1987; Blanchard,

Hori, and Blanchard 1987). Psychomotor stimulants such as amphetamine also increase attack behavior. In contrast, high doses of alcohol reduce aggression (Miczek et al. 1989). Taken together, these findings suggest that the low-dose stimulatory effects of alcohol (discussed previously with reference to the biphasic actions of alcohol) may, in some situations, be associated with increased aggression. It is interesting that, in a naturalistic social colony group, subordinate male rats voluntarily drink more alcohol solution than dominant males, thus suggesting that the social stress of subordination may be a factor in alcohol consumption (Blanchard, Hori et al. 1987).

Aversive Effects of Alcohol

At least some of the postingestional effects of alcohol are aversive or dysphoric, especially when high doses are administered. The aversive properties of alcohol are relevant to alcohol abuse for two reasons: First, they may place an upper limit on the amount of alcohol that can be consumed, and second, tolerance to the aversive effects of alcohol may account for increases in alcohol intake over time (Cappell and LeBlanc 1981). These ideas are supported by the observation that when alcohol-naive animals are tested, selectively bred P rats are less sensitive to the aversive effects of alcohol than NP rats (Froehlich et al. 1988). Furthermore, the aversive effects of alcohol are attenuated in P rats with a history of oral alcohol self-administration, thereby suggesting that tolerance had developed to the aversive effects of alcohol during chronic drinking (Stewart et al. 1991). This tolerance, as well as an innate reduced sensitivity to the aversive effects of alcohol, could contribute to the high alcohol intake shown by P rats.

The aversive effects of alcohol have been most often studied in rats by using the conditioned taste aversion procedure (figure 4). In this procedure, the consumption of a drug-free fluid with a novel taste is associated with a drug treatment and aversion to the drug's effects are inferred from the animal's subsequent avoidance of that flavor. A related procedure involves associating the effects of a drug with environmental cues rather than with a flavor (e.g., Cunningham 1979). Several physiological features may be involved in the aversive effects of alcohol. For example, alcohol interferes with body temperature regulation (Cunningham et al. 1988); in addition, alterations in the GABA-benzodiazepine receptor complex may be involved in mediating alcohol's

Figure 4. The conditioned taste aversion procedure used to measure aversion to a drug treatment in rats.

During the initial trial, rats drink a sweet saccharin solution for 15 minutes. Then the rats are injected immediately with either saline (the control group) or the agent being tested (the drug group). On another day, the saccharin is again presented for 15 minutes to the same rats. The saline-injected rats continue to drink the saccharin. However, the drug-treated rats now avoid the saccharin, indicating that they associate the flavor of the solution with the aversive effects of the drug. They avoid drinking a fluid if their consumption of it was followed by ill effects.

[blocks in formation]

effects on behavior (Jeffreys et al. 1990; Smith
et al. 1989). Alcohol aversion does not seem to
depend on the detection of alcohol as a toxin or
poison in the blood; rather, it appears to be due
to the drug's direct effects in the brain (Rabin
et al. 1989; Stewart et al. 1988). Whether alcohol
produces aversive or rewarding effects also de-
pends on the nature of the animals' history of
alcohol administration and whether the rats self-
administer the alcohol or receive it passively by
injection (Gauvin and Holloway 1992).

When alcohol is metabolized in the liver, acet-
aldehyde is produced as a byproduct. Because
the administration of acetaldehyde is also aver-
sive, the aversive effects of alcohol may be sec-
ondary to the production of acetaldehyde
(Aragon et al. 1991). An important theoretical

question is whether alcohol-dependent persons drink excessively because the drug is more reinforcing, less aversive, or both.

Polydrug Abuse

Implicit in the suggestion that brain reward systems affect alcohol reinforcement is the hypothesis that those systems may also reinforce other classes of drugs (Wise 1980). Alcohol may interact with different drugs in different ways. For example, rats with a history of alcohol drinking have been found to readily self-administer the benzodiazepine chlordiazepoxide (Librium), but rats with a history of cocaine self-administration do not (Falk and Tang 1989). This finding is

consistent with the idea that alcohol abusers may also abuse other sedative drugs. A second kind of interaction occurs when two or more drugs are taken together. For example, the combination of alcohol and pentobarbital appears to be synergistic, that is, the combination is more reinforcing than either drug alone (Meisch and Lemaire 1990); a similar effect is observed for the combination of alcohol and morphine (Marglin et al. 1988). Cocaine and alcohol are frequently used together by humans. The administration of these drugs together results in an interactive facilitation of BSR in rats (Lewis 1985) that may be mediated by ethylcocaine (cocaethylene), an active metabolite of cocaine and alcohol (Dean et al. 1991; Jatlow et al. 1991; see Chapter 7, Biochemical Effects of Alcohol Metabolism). This byproduct of the joint use of alcohol and cocaine may function as a reinforcer, thus increasing the motivation to use both drugs again. Because many abused drugs share common mechanisms of action, the future discovery of other similar interactions seems likely.

Summary

Substantial progress is being made in elucidating many of the neural systems responsible for alcohol reinforcement. These advances in turn are beginning to lead to the development of pharmacological agents that offer the potential to reduce alcohol drinking in animal models. As a direct result of this research, medications aimed at reducing excessive alcohol drinking are beginning to be tested in humans. Ample evidence suggests that alcohol reinforcement is a physiological or biological process. The work with selectively bred animal lines also suggests that the degree of alcohol reinforcement is heritable.

References

Altshuler, H.L.; Phillips, P.E,; and Feinhandler, D.A. Alterations of ethanol self-administration by naltrexone. Life Sci 26:679–688, 1980.

Amit, Z.; Gill, K.; and Ng Cheong Ton, J.M. Attenuation of voluntary ethanol consumption by dopamine-beta-hydroxylase inhibition (FLA-57): Mediated by changes in aversion, reinforcement, or both. Alcohol 8(2):79–85, 1991.

Aragon, C.M.; Abitbol, M.; and Amit, Z. Ethanolinduced CTA mediated by acetaldehyde through central catecholamine activity. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 103(1):74–77, 1991.

Bain, G.T., and Kornetsky, C. Ethanol oral selfadministration and rewarding brain stimulation. Alcohol 6(6):499–503, 1989.

Balakleevsky, A.; Colombo, G.; Fadda, F.; and
Gessa, G.L. RO 19-4603, a benzodiazepine
receptor inverse agonist, attenuates voluntary
ethanol consumption in rats selectively bred
for high ethanol preference. Alcohol Alcohol
25(5):449-452, 1990.

Baldwin, H.A.; Wall, T.L.; Schuckit, M.A.; and
Koob, G.F. Differential effects of ethanol on pun-
ished responding in the P and NP rats. Alcohol
Clin Exp Res 15(4):700–704, 1991.

Bice, P.J., and Kiefer, S.W. Taste reactivity in alcohol preferring and nonpreferring rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 14(5):721-727, 1990.

Blanchard, R.J.; Blanchard, D.C.; and Weiss, S.M.

Ethanol effects in an anxiety/defense test battery. Alcohol 7(5):375–381, 1990. Blanchard, D.C.; Flannelly, K.; Hori, K.; and

Blanchard, R.J. Ethanol effects on female aggression vary with opponent size and time within session. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 27(4):645648, 1987.

Blanchard, R.J.; Hori, K.; and Blanchard, D.C. Ethanol effects on aggression of rats selected for different levels of aggressiveness. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 27(4):641–644, 1987. Blanchard, R.J.; Hori, K.; Tom, P.; and Blanchard, D.C. Social structure and ethanol consumption in the laboratory rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 28(4):437-442, 1987.

Cappell, H., and Herman, C.P. Alcohol and tension reduction: A review. QJ Stud Alcohol 33(1):33– 64, 1972.

Cappell, H., and LeBlanc, A.E. Tolerance and physical dependence: Do they play a role in alcohol and drug self-administration? In: Israel, Y.; Glaser, F.B.; Kalant, H.; Popham, R.E.; Schmidt, W.; and Smart, R.G., eds. Research Advances in Alcohol and Drug Problems. Vol 6. New York: Plenum Press, 1981. pp. 159–196.

Carboni, E.; Acquas, E.; Frau, R.; and Di Chiara, G. Differential inhibitory effects of a 5-HT3 antagonist on drug-induced stimulation of dopamine release. Eur J Pharmacol 164(3):515–519, 1989. Carroll, M.E., and Meisch, R.A. Increased drugreinforced behavior due to food deprivation. In: Thompson, T.; Dews, P.B.; and Barret, J.E., eds. Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology. Vol 4. New York: Academic Press, 1984. pp. 47-88. Carroll, M.E.; Stitzer, M.L.; Strain, E.; and Meisch, R.A. The behavioral pharmacology of alcohol and other drugs: Emerging issues. In: Galanter,

M., ed. Recent Developments in Alcoholism. Vol 8. New York: Plenum Press, 1990. pp. 5–46. Chopin, P., and Briley, M. Animal models of anxi

ety: The effect of compounds that modify 5-HT
neurotransmission. Trends Pharmocol Sci
8(10):383-388, 1987.

Cloninger, C.R. Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms
in alcoholism. Science 236:410–416, 1987.
Crabbe, J.C.; Young, E.R.; Deutsch, C.M.; Tam, B.R.;
and Kosobud, A. Mice genetically selected for
differences in open-field activity after ethanol.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 27:577-581, 1987.
Crowley, T.J.; Williams, E.A.; and Jones, R.H. Initiat-
ing ethanol drinking in a simian social group in
a naturalistic setting. Alcohol Clin Exp Res
14(3):444-455, 1990.

Cunningham, C.L. Flavor and location aversions
produced by ethanol. Behav Neural Biol
27:362–367, 1979.

Cunningham, C.L.; Hawks, D.M.; and Niehus, D.R.
Role of hypothermia in ethanol-induced condi-
tioned taste aversion. Psychopharmacology
(Berl) 95(3):318–322, 1988.

Dalterio, S.L.; Wayner, M.J.; Geller, I.; and Hart

mann, R.J. Ethanol and diazepam interactions
on conflict behavior in rats. Alcohol 5(6):471-
476, 1988.

Daoust, M.; Protais, P.; and Ladure, P. Noradrenergic system: Effect of DSP4 and FLA-57 on ethanol intake in ethanol-preferring rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 36(1):133–137, 1990.

Dean, R.A.; Christian, C.D.; Barry Sample, R.H.; and Bosron, W.F. Human liver cocaine esterases: Ethanol-mediated formation of ethylcocaine. FASEB J 5(12):2735–2739, 1991.

Deneau, G.; Yanagita, T.; and Seevers, M.H. Selfadministration of psychoactive substances by the monkey. Psychopharmacologia 16(1):30– 48, 1969.

De Vry, J., and Slangen, J.L. Effects of training dose on discrimination and cross-generalization of chlordiazepoxide, pentobarbital, and ethanol in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 88(3):341345, 1986.

Di Chiara, G., and Imperato, A. Drugs abused by humans preferentially increase synaptic dopamine concentrations in the mesolimbic system of freely moving rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85(14):5274-5278, 1988.

Dudek, B.C.; Phillips, T.J.; and Hahn, M.E. Genetic

analyses of the biphasic nature of the alcohol
dose-response curve. Alcohol Clin Exp Res
15(2):262-269, 1991.

Dunn, R.W.; Corbett, R.; and Fielding, S. Effects of 5-HT1A receptor agonists and NMDA receptor

antagonists in the social interaction test and the elevated plus maze. Eur J Pharmacol 169(1):1– 10, 1989.

Durcan, M.J., and Lister, R.G. Time course of ethanol's effects on locomotor activity, exploration, and anxiety in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 96:67-72, 1988.

Durcan, M.J.; Lister, R.G.; Eckardt, M.J.; and Linnoila, M. Behavioral interactions of fluoxetine and other 5-hydroxytryptamine uptake inhibitors with ethanol in tests of anxiety, locomotion, and exploration. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 96(4):528–533, 1988.

Eddy, N.B.; Halbach, H.; Isbell, H.; and Seevers,
M.H. Drug dependence: Its significance and
characteristics. Bull World Health Organ
32(5):721-733, 1965.

Emmett-Oglesby, M.W. Tolerance to the discriminative effects of ethanol. Behav Pharmacol 1:497– 503, 1990.

Eriksson, K. Genetic selection for voluntary alcohol consumption in the albino rat. Science 159:739– 741, 1968.

Fadda, F.; Garau, B.; Marchei, F.; Colombo, G.; and Gessa, G.L. MDL 72222, a selective 5-HT3 receptor antagonist, suppresses voluntary ethanol consumption in alcohol-preferring rats. Alcohol Alcohol 26(2):107-110, 1991.

Fadda, F.; Mosca, E.; Colombo, G.; and Gessa, G.L. Effect of spontaneous ingestion of ethanol on brain dopamine metabolism. Life Sci 44(4):281287, 1989.

Fadda, F.; Mosca, E.; Colombo, G.; and Gessa, G.L. Alcohol-preferring rats: Genetic sensitivity to alcohol-induced stimulation of dopamine metabolism. Physiol Behav 47(4):727-729, 1990. Falk, J.L., and Tang, M. Schedule-induced chlordiazepoxide intake: Differential effect of cocaine and ethanol histories. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 33(2):393–396, 1989. Files, F.J.; Andrews, C.M.; Samson, H.H.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K. Alcohol self-administration in a non-restricted access situation with alcoholpreferring (P) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res, in press. Friedman, H.J.; Carpenter, J.A.; Lester, D.; and Randall, C.L. Effect of alpha-methyl-p-tyrosine on dose-dependent mouse strain differences in locomotor activity after ethanol. J Stud Alcohol 41(1):1-7, 1980.

Froehlich, J.C.; Harts, J.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K. Naloxone attenuation of voluntary alcohol consumption. Advances in Biomedical Alcohol Research: Third Congress of the International Society for Biomedical Research on Alcoholism,

Helsinki, June 13-18, 1987. Alcohol Alcohol Suppl 1:333-337, 1987. Froehlich, J.C.; Harts, J.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K. Differences in response to the aversive properties of ethanol in rats selectively bred for oral ethanol preference. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 31(1):215–222, 1988.

Froehlich, J.C.; Harts, J.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K.

Naloxone attenuates voluntary ethanol intake in
rats selectively bred for high ethanol preference.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 35(2):385–390,
1990.

Froehlich, J.C.; Zweifel, M.; Harts, J.; Lumeng, L.;
and Li, T.-K. Importance of delta opioid recep-
tors in maintaining high alcohol drinking. Psy-
chopharmacology (Berl) 103(4):467-472, 1991.
Frye, G.D., and Breese, G.R. An evaluation of the
locomotor stimulating action of ethanol in rats
and mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl)
75(4):372-379, 1981.

Gatto, G.J.; Murphy, J.M.; McBride, W.J.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K. Intracranial self-administration of ethanol into the ventral tegmental area of alcohol-preferring (P) rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 14:291, 1990.

Gatto, G.J.; Murphy, J.M.; Waller, M.B.; McBride, W.J.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K. Persistence of tolerance to a single dose of ethanol in the selectively bred alcohol-preferring P rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 28(1):105–110, 1987. Gauvin, D.V.; Harland, R.D.; Criado, J.R.; Michaelis, R.C.; and Holloway, F.A. The discriminative stimulus properties of ethanol and acute ethanol withdrawal states in rats. Drug Alcohol Depend 24(2):103–113, 1989.

Gauvin, D.V., and Holloway, F.A. Historical factors in the development of ETOH-conditioned place preference. Alcohol 9(1):1-7, 1992.

George, F.R. The role of arachidonic acid metabolites in mediating ethanol self-administration and intoxication. Ann NY Acad Sci 559:382391, 1989.

Gessa, G.L.; Muntoni, F.; Collu, M.; Vargiu, L.;

and Mereu, G. Low doses of ethanol activate dopaminergic neurons in the ventral tegmental area. Brain Res 348(1):201–203, 1985. Gianoulakis, C.; Angelogianni, P.; Meany, M.; Thavundayil, J.; and Tawar, V. Endorphins in individuals with high and low risk for development of alcoholism. In: Reid, L.D., ed. Opioids, Bulimia, and Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. New York: Springer-Verlag, 1990. pp. 229–247. Gill, K., and Amit, Z. Serotonin uptake blockers

and voluntary alcohol consumption: A review of recent studies. In: Galanter, M., ed. Recent De

velopments in Alcoholism. Vol. 7. New York: Plenum Press, 1989. pp. 225–248. Gongwer, M.A.; Murphy, J.M.; McBride, W.J.; Lumeng, L.; and Li, T.-K. Regional brain contents of serotonin, dopamine, and their metabolites in the selectively bred high- and low-alcohol drinking lines of rats. Alcohol 6(4):317-320, 1989. Grant, K.A., and Barrett, J.E. Blockade of the discriminative stimulus effects of ethanol with 5HT3 receptor antagonists. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 104(4):451–456, 1991.

Grant, K.A.; Knisely, J.S.; Tabakoff, B.; Barrett, J.E.; and Balster, R.L. Ethanol-like discriminative stimulus effects of noncompetitive N-methyl-Daspartate antagonists. Behav Pharmacol 2:87– 95, 1991.

Grupp, L.A.; Kalant, H.; and Leenen, F.H.H. Alcohol intake is inversely related to plasma renin activity in the genetically selected alcoholpreferring and -nonpreferring lines of rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 32(4):1061–1063, 1989.

Grupp, L.A.; Perlanski, E.; and Stewart, R.B. Regulation of alcohol consumption by the reninangiotensin system: A review of recent findings and a possible mechanism of action. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 15(2):265-275, 1991.

Haraguchi, M.; Samson, H.H.; and Tolliver, G.A. Reduction in oral ethanol self-administration in the rat by the 5-HT uptake blocker fluoxetine. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 35(1):259–262, 1990. Higley, J.D.; Hasert, M.F.; Suomi, S.J.; and Linnoila, M. Nonhuman primate model of alcohol abuse: Effects of early experience, personality, and stress on alcohol consumption. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 88(16):7261–7265, 1991.

Hiltunen, A.J., and Jarbe, T.U.C. RO 15-4513 does not antagonize the discriminative stimulus- or rate-depressant effects of ethanol in rats. Alcohol 5(3):203-207, 1988.

Hwang, B.H.; Lumeng, L.; Wu, J.Y.; and Li, T.-K. Increased number of GABAergic terminals in the nucleus accumbens is associated with alcohol preference in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 14(4):503–507, 1990.

Jaffe, J.H. Drug addiction and drug abuse. In: Gilman, A.G.; Goodman, L.S.; Rall, T.W.; and Murad, F., eds. The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics. 7th ed. New York: Macmillan, 1985. pp. 532-581.

Jarbe, T.U.C., and McMillan, D.E. Interaction of the discriminative stimulus properties of diazepam and ethanol in pigeons. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 18(1):73-80, 1983.

« ПредыдущаяПродолжить »